All Talks

Cognitive effects of possessive classifiers: visualising complex data with dendrograms

Do the nominal classification systems found in Oceanic languages have an impact on cognition? Many Oceanic languages have a set of possessive classifiers that are used to categorise alienable possessions according to how the possessor intends to use these possessions. Data from a suite of experiments were collected from six Oceanic languages spoken in Vanuatu and New Caledonia: Merei, Lewo, Vatlongos, North Ambrym, Nêlêmwa and Iaai. The inventory of possessive classifiers for these languages ranges from two to 24. Typically, a noun can occur with different classifiers, depending on how the possessed item is used by the possessor (Lichtenberk 1983). For example, wi ‘water’ in Lewo (Vanuatu) occurs with either the drinkable or the general classifier.

1a. ma-na wi
CL.DRINK-3SG water
‘her (drinking) water’
1b. sa-na wi
CL.GEN-3SG water
‘her (washing) water’

In marked contrast, North Ambrym’s (Vanuatu) cognate for water – we – occurs only with the drinkable classifier (2a), not the general classifier (2b):

2a. ma-n we
CL.DRINK-3SG water
‘his/her water (for any purpose)’
2b. mwena-n we
CL.GEN-3SG water
intended:‘her water’

We argue that North Ambrym’s innovative system resembles a gender system: a noun occurs with a particular classifier regardless of contextual interactions. This leads to two questions (RQ1) do more gender-like fixed assignment systems (e.g. North Ambrym) effect cognition in ways which differ from systems with a more classifier-like system (e.g. Lewo)? And (RQ2) do systems with larger classifier inventories effect cognition differently (e.g. Iaai with 24 classifiers) than systems with fewer classifiers (e.g. Merei with two classifiers)?

We discuss the results from a card sorting experiment. Participants sorted pictures of possessed entities into groups in two ways: (i) a free sort, where participants put the pictures into as many piles as they wanted according to perceived similarity; (ii) a structured sort, where participants were asked to group the images according to which classifier they could occur with. If possessive classifiers have an impact on cognition we expect to find similar groupings of pictures across the two tasks.

We explore the resulting data visually using hierarchical agglomerative clustering. This type of cluster analysis produces trees called dendrograms, which are a heuristic aid to explore categorisation data (Borcard et al. 2011:63). We present dendrograms for our experimental data and discuss ways of exploring the clusters. Fig. 1 shows a dendrogram for the structured sort for Lewo: its three possessive classifiers – drink, food and general – are represented by three distinct clusters. We outline the different statistical methods that make use of heuristic data exploration to compare clusters across different dendrograms (Robinson & Foulds 1981; Fowlkes & Mallows 1983; Lapointe & Legendre 1995). The value of these methods is that they help us to establish how far the groupings made in the free sort (i) are similar to those made in the structured sort (ii). We then discuss how our findings bear on our RQs on the relationship between classifier categories and cognition.
Dendrogram representing the structured sort for Lewo
References
Borcard, D., F. Gillet & P. Legendre. (2011). Numerical Ecology with R. NY: Springer.
Fowlkes, E. B., & C. L. Mallows (1983). A method for comparing two hierarchical clusterings. Journal of the American statistical association, 78(383), 553-569.
Lapointe, F-J. & P. Legendre. (1995). Comparison tests for dendrograms: A comparative evaluation. Journal of Classification, 12(2), 265-282.
Lichtenberk, F. 1983. Relational classifiers. Lingua 60(2-3):147–176.
Robinson, D. F. & L. R. Foulds. (1981). Comparison of phylogenetic trees. Mathematical Biosciences, 53(1-2), 131–147.

All Talks